TU-L0022 - Statistical Research Methods D, Lecture, 2.11.2021-6.4.2022
This course space end date is set to 06.04.2022 Search Courses: TU-L0022
Randomized experiments (6:45)
This video explains how randomized experiments are used to make causal claims.
Click to view transcript
The first strategy for making causal claims using quantitative data is a randomized experiment. The idea of randomized experiment is that we have some population of interest from which we take a sample. Then we divide the sample into two randomly. One is called the treatment group and another one is the control group.Because we select those two groups randomly there are no differences statistically between these two groups. Or if there are some differences then it's due to chance only.This
relates to back to our example of dividing the men and women-led
companies into two groups randomly to see if there's a difference. We divide these in to two groups with treatment and control. Then we apply some kind of treatment to the treatment group.Typically
this example is from medical research so this is applied in medicine
and it's easy to understand. One group receives a pill, the other one
doesn't. Then after, let's say two days, we assume that the effect takes
two days to be realized. We
measure the health of these two groups, we compare if the group that
received the pill, the medicine is better than the second group. Then we
conclude that there's a causal effect. The why this is
a valid causal claim is that these groups are perfectly comparable to
start with because they are randomly chosen from the same sample.Therefore
the only plausible explanation beyond chance for the difference between
the groups is that there is an actual effect of the treatment. This
works well under certain conditions. So we need to have a random
assignment that's very important. If we have people who get to choose
whether they receive the medicine or not, then those people who are more
sick will likely to choose to be in the treatment group than the
control group. And then comparison here would confound the selection
effect of how people chose to be in these groups and the treatment
effect. Then we have a large enough
sample. And then some other assumptions that are not as relevant. We
have large enough sample that we don't have to worry about chance, we
have random assignment here and after that we can compare the difference
after receiving the medicine or the treatment as causal effect. The
randomization is important because we want to show that this difference
is because of the treatment and not because we chose to assign the
groups in a certain way. We want to show that there is the treatment
effect instead of a selection effect. Then
we repeat this a couple of times and when the study results have been
verified independently or two times, then we can sell our medicine. And
that's how randomized experiments work. Of course there are variations
to this design like you can compare how health of an individual
increases, so that would be a within individual study. This is a between
individual study but this is the base case. This is the simplest
possible experimental design. Experimental
designs are not always feasible they can be done in business studies
but if we study organizations then appliying treatments to organizations
could be difficult to organize. We
also have a second best option called Quasi-experiment and the idea of a
quasi-experiment is that we have some elements of experimental
approach, but we don't have the full experimental control. For
example we could have separate sample pretest and post-test. We have
something for example, we know that we have a school and the kids will
receive a medicine. Everyone gets the medicine on one day, but we can't
influence that. What we can do is
that we randomize the kids, we measure their health for half of the
students before the treatment for other half after the treatment. And
then we assume that this after the treatment group is otherwise
comparable for the before the treatment group except for the treatment.So we assume that there are no time effects. And that would allow us to make a causal claim based on quasi-experimental design. We
can also have experiments where the choice between treatment and
control is not random. Either it would look like random we don't have
control on the randomization in which case we would assume that these
samples behave as if they were random samples. Or we can do some
statistical adjustments for this non-random selection. So that's
nonequivalent control group design. Another
one is interrupted time series design. So we follow some units or some
companies, people over time. Then there is an exogenous shock that
happens, so some kind of exogenous event. For example new regulation is
implemented in markets independently of these organizations. Then we can
analyze what is the effect of that new regulation on company
performance. Assuming that the implementation doesn't in any way depend
on how these companies are doing. That's
another quasi-experimental design. So the idea of quasi-experimental
design is that we have a treatment but we don't have the full
randomization. So something happens, something is manipulated but we
don't really have quite a full experimental design. Quasi-experiments
are something that people overlook when they think about their designs.
There's a great article in Organizational Research Methods about
different quasi-experimental designs. I would recommend that you
consider these when designing your studies because you can make really
strong claims that are perhaps more generalizable than lab experiments,
because quasi-experiments typically take place in real-life settings.